Aphorisms


There's nothing so bad, that adding government can't make it worse. -- The Immigrant

Government is not the solution to our problem; government is the problem. -- Ronald Reagan

*******
Read the next two together:

Every collectivist revolution rides in on a Trojan horse of 'Emergency'." -- Herbert Hoover

This is too good a crisis to waste. -- Rahm Emanuel

*******
Government is the great fiction through which everybody endeavors to live at the expense of everybody else. -- Fredric Bastiat, French Economist (30 June 1801 – 24 December 1850)

In general, the art of government consists of taking as much money as possible from one party of the citizens to give to another. -- François-Marie Arouet, a.k.a. Voltaire, (21 November 1694 – 30 May 1778)

The problem with socialism is that, sooner or later, you run out of other people's money. -- Margaret Thatcher

The inherent vice of capitalism is the unequal sharing of blessings; the inherent virtue of socialism is the equal sharing of miseries. -- Winston Churchill

Saturday, September 19, 2009

#25: Evolution (Yet Again!)

Most of the European 18th century was fascinated with “natural history” and with the question of speciation. Yes, this was an important issue long before Darwin came upon the scene. It became important almost immediately once the Aristotelian and Christian world view of fixed species came into question. One interesting thesis was that species are a kind of illusion, that in fact there exist creatures who imperceptibly fill the apparent gaps between the different kinds with which we are familiar. We can see the beginnings of evolutionary thinking in the writings of David Hume. An explicit effort to grapple with the problem came from Jean-Baptiste Lamarck (1 August 1744 – 18 December 1829) who hypothesized that acquired characteristics were heritable. This was the not-so-good evolutionary theory. Thus, Federer’s skills as a tennis player would automatically be transferred to his progeny. None of this theorizing hit the jackpot until Darwin’s On The Origin of Species came out in 1859. The huge advantage of this theory came from it’s providing a purely mechanical and intuitively satisfying explanation of the appearance of new species, namely the doctrine of “natural selection.”

In the interest of fairness, and not to detract from Darwin’s achievement, I should mention that the doctrine of natural selection already appeared in Lucretius’s (ca. 99 BC- ca. 55 BC) De Rerum Natura (the passage is too long to cite, but the interested reader should look at the section entitled “Origins of Vegetable and Animal Life” in any edition of the poem).

Now as I said, the notion of natural selection is indeed intuitively satisfying, but this might in fact be a burden for the theory. The reason I say this is that the attractiveness of the doctrine may well be responsible for a kind of carelessness with respect to detail. In particular, I have a problem with the theory that I am hoping one of our readers will successfully address. I suspect that I’m missing something obvious, but try as I might, I still don’t have an answer to my puzzle. Here is my problem.

Here is the theory of speciation by natural selection as I understand it. We assume a limited amount of food and space to be available to a population of creatures. We further assume that the creatures of this population compete for the food and space. We thirdly assume that those creatures less well equipped in the fight for survival die in greater numbers and sooner than their better equipped competitors. We finally assume that the creatures are subject to random mutations, some of which improve their chances in the battle for survival and others do not.

We can easily see the intuitive attractiveness of this idea if we consider a fictitious population of raptors alike in all respects, except that some of them have eyes and the rest do not. It seems immediately plausible to expect the eyed ones to prevail and the blind ones to die out. The eyes ones would find the food before the blind ones, the eyed ones would find the strong, good-looking raptor women before the blind ones, and in conflict, the eyed ones would presumably beat the blind ones. After not too long a time, the only remaining raptors of this group would be eyed. So goes the theory of natural selection.

The problem with this story, for me, is that it works really well if we assume a fully functioning eye as the adaptation, but not so much if we assume a non-functional transitional eye still in the process of “evolving.”

That is: if the theory of natural selection forces us to hypothesize instantaneous mutations introducing fully functional eyes (or whichever organ you prefer), then its initial intuitive attractiveness disappears.

Having a fully functional eye involves an enormous amount of structural change on the organism’s part. The eye itself is very complicated, but, in addition, in order for the eye to be functional, the brain must also be altered and there has to exist appropriate neurological connection between eye and brain. It stretches probability to the breaking point to assume that one of the raptors is born complete with such an enormous and functional mutation. It doubles the stretch of this probability to yet further assume this raptor finds another one like itself with whom to breed a race of eyed raptors.

What is far more likely is evolution is a kind of creeping adaptive change. What I mean is that one or a few of the raptors are born with one or a few new cells where eyes will eventually be. If we can figure out 1) why these raptors will breed with enough other raptors like themselves to eventually created a “new cell” population, and 2) why these raptors are better at surviving than their fellows, we might be in position to see how eventually an eyed population could evolve. But the stages that precede being eyed do not bring any survival advantage with them – it is sight, and only sight, that is an aid in the contest for survival. Until the collection of new cells actually confers sight on the raptor that owns them, it is in exactly the same position as all the others, no better at surviving and no worse.

The theory works on the assumption that the population ratio of more adaptive to less adaptive changes in favor of the adaptive group, which is why there is more adaptive-adaptive reproducing and why eventually there are only the adaptive ones left. But there is no reason to assume this ratio will change during the period before the eye actually functions and gives the creature an adaptive advantage. Which entails that no uniformly improved population will be produced on which yet further mutations will occur to eventually produce a functional eye.

In a nutshell, then, I cannot understand how the evolutionary process, i.e. natural selection, works during the interim period between no eye at all, on the one hand, and a functional eye, on the other.

The only way I can see the theory working is with some ad hoc assuming.

If we assume that pre-functional eye cells confer some other adaptive advantage over the creatures that have it, then the rest of the process can be taken to work. If, for example, pre-functional-eye cells have the ability to “sense” danger in some manner that doesn’t involve great neurological or structural complexity, then they could give a competitive advantage to their owners. Perhaps there are yet other possibilities of this kind. The problem would lie in finding some kind of empirical evidence that supports such an auxiliary hypothesis.

3 comments:

  1. I think the answer to your dilemma lies with what you described as 'creeping', or what could be called incremental evolutionary change. An organism that senses the outside environment better than it's peers would have an advantage, whether that sensation is fully realized sight, or another form. Such sensory organs might continue to evolve over generations to become eyes. One can see a similar phenomenon within the political development of the Left, wherein a given player could initially gain advantage by organizing collective resources to leverage the power of 'socialism'. Over time, such skills would be rewarded and now, they have evolved the ability to coerce the English language, mutate the founding father's vision of government, and propagate myths about global warming, all of which confers substantial political gain and propogation of the Left species (-;

    ReplyDelete
  2. Thanks for your comments, Shaqfinance. I don't know that you solve my problem, since it seems to be to reappear no matter how small the increment. It seems to me that the movement from NO helpful, adaptive organ TO a functional one (no matter how small the function) MUST involve steps with no increase in utility. If this is the case, then at those points, NO change in the ratio within the population will occur and no increase in better adapted creatures. The whole of the Darwinian theory seems premised on this ONE thing, namely that an increase in adaptability yields an increase of the population WITH the improved adaptability (relative to the whole population). Thus, if the process of evolution includes links at which the population does not change in this way, the process should simply stop. Or so it seems.

    ReplyDelete
  3. Simplicius,

    The eye is one common question for the evolutionary theorist. As such, a well thought out answer can be offered. As long as you can picture how one surface cell could adapt photo sensitivity the rest of the picture develops naturally. I will start at the stage of single cell photo-sensitivity of a cell that already had a reflexive loop with a population of muscle cells (such as a a withdrawl reflex to increases in pressure). Now if this cell gained photo-sensitivity through a random mutation in the reproductive cells and it was in an organism, lets say at the level of a sea slug, that might benefit from either going away from or towards light, it could cause a adaptive change in the organism.

    This single cell through evolution could have become a group of cells. This group of cells could have evolved to become a concave disc (higher detection of motion) and so on and so forth. This video shows a lot of the theoretical steps.
    (http://www.pbs.org/wgbh/evolution/library/01/1/l_011_01.html)

    I think one of the big questions that you have asked is, "how could an adaptive change like photosensitivity be immediately plugged into a nervous system." This is a huge issue if you are thinking about a Raptor. However, the first photoreceptive cell didn't evolve in the raptor, it evolved in a organism without such complexity. If you think about very basic nervous systems in which receptor cells connect directly to muscle cells then the adaption of a response to light is not so hard to imagine.

    As for the speciation question, I am not sure how your example relates. Speciation occurs through a number of different mechanisms which cause mutations that prevent members of a shared common species from mating. This could be as simple as some members adapting the ability to live in new habitats so that they no longer have the chance to mate with there fellow members in a different habitat. Speciation can also occur if an environmental event like a volcano or the formation of a river separates members of a species. The then separate members have different environmental factors that select different qualities. Eventually they are so different that even if they come back into contact they can not mate. The mechanism you discussed is one I have heard of. Mutation within one lineage two species emerge with two separate niches. I think the trick there is that the new mutation not just be a more adaptive version of the species within the same niche. In that case the old mutation carries would just disappear.

    Jeff

    ReplyDelete